|

Boyfriends, Girlfriends and Teenagers' Risk of Sexual Involvement

Barbara VanOss Marín Douglas B. Kirby Esther S. Hudes Karin K. Coyle Cynthia A. Gómez

First published online:

| DOI: https://doi.org/10.1363/3807606
Abstract / Summary
CONTEXT

Having a boyfriend or girlfriend, especially an older one, is associated with increased sexual risk in early adolescence. The mechanisms underlying this association are unclear.

METHODS

Middle school students in Northern California were surveyed annually from 1997 to 2000. For a sample of 1,214 males and 1,308 females who were sexually inexperienced in seventh grade, logistic and linear regression were used to explore associations between relationship status in seventh grade and sexual activity in ninth grade, controlling for sixth-grade and eighth-grade characteristics.

RESULTS

Males who had had a girlfriend their age by seventh grade were more likely than those who had had no relationship to report sexual activity in ninth grade (odds ratio, 2.1). Similarly, for females, the odds of being sexually active in ninth grade were elevated among those who had had a boyfriend their age (2.9); however, they also were higher among those who had had an older boyfriend than among those who had had one their age (2.1). With sixth-grade risk factors controlled, relationship status in seventh grade remained significant only for females; the association was explained by early menarche and by participation in situations that could lead to sex and riskier peer norms in eighth grade. For males, eighth-grade situations that could lead to sex, Hispanic ethnicity and sixth-grade peer norms explained ninth-grade sexual behavior.

CONCLUSIONS

To reduce the risk of adolescent sexual activity, parents and communities should encourage youth in middle school, especially females who experience early menarche, to delay serious romantic relationships.

Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 2006, 38(2):76–83

Various problems arise when young females have older male partners. For example, sexual relationships between females aged 15 or younger and males three or more years their senior are less likely to be voluntary, less likely to involve contraceptive use and more likely to result in pregnancy than relationships between young adolescents who are close in age.1 And high proportions of babies born to teenage mothers were fathered by adult men;2 these teenage mothers are more likely to be involved in problem behaviors than are their counterparts with similar-age partners.3

In addition, having an older boyfriend may increase women's risk of acquiring an STD. For example, among 258 sexually active young women attending an adolescent health clinic, having a partner two or more years older was associated with increased odds of having an STD at screening.4 Similarly, in a sample of pregnant black teenagers, the chances of testing positive for chlamydia were four times as high, and the chances of reporting that their partner had other partners were twice as high, among those with older partners as among those with same-age partners; young women with older partners also reported almost twice the number of unprotected sexual encounters as those with partners closer in age.5

Elevated rates of pregnancy, childbearing and STD among young women with older partners are not surprising, because these women initiate sexual activity earlier and use contraceptives less frequently than others.6

The effects of having an older boyfriend on sexual behavior are most pronounced among the youngest teenagers.7 In the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health), the odds of having intercourse were six times as high among 13-year-olds with older boyfriends as among those with boyfriends their own age; for 17-year-olds, by contrast, the odds were only doubled.8 In the 2002 National Survey of Family Growth (NSFG), 37% of women who had first had sex before age 14 had had a first male partner who was four or more years their senior, compared with 16% of those whose first intercourse had occurred at ages 17–19.9 In a state survey in Vermont, 34% of females who had first had sex at age 11–12 had had a first partner who was five or more years older, compared with only 7% of those whose first sex occurred at ages 16–18.10

Having a boyfriend of any age is associated with an increased likelihood of sexual initiation. In one study of Add Health participants in grades 7–12, the most important predictor of sexual experience (among a set of 50) was having been in a romantic relationship during the previous 18 months.11 Other studies using the same data set but different analytic methods have reached similar conclusions.12

Evidence is mixed on the effect of having a girlfriend, especially an older one, on males. In a study of mostly non-Hispanic white eighth graders, having a girlfriend was not associated with males' initiation of sex.13 However, in a sample of sixth graders from largely Hispanic middle schools, males with a same-age or older girlfriend were significantly more likely than those with no girlfriend to have had sex, even when peer norms and unwanted sexual advances were controlled for.14 In the Add Health sample, regardless of race or ethnicity, the most important risk factor for initiation of sex for males (as for females) was having been in a romantic relationship.15

While having a boyfriend or girlfriend, particularly an older one, is associated with sexual initiation, relatively little is known about how or why this happens. Several mechanisms can be hypothesized. Having a boyfriend or girlfriend may increase the opportunity for engaging in precoital behaviors, which may be followed by sex.16 Furthermore, having a boyfriend or girlfriend may expose a youth to a new set of friends, who may share more permissive norms about sex, especially if they are older; many studies have demonstrated that youth whose peer norms encourage sexual activity have an increased likelihood of being sexually active.17 Because young people see having a boyfriend or girlfriend as desirable,18 pleasing that person and that person's friends is probably important to many youth.

Also, some youth experience coercion to have sex. The youngest teenagers frequently report that their first intercourse was nonvoluntary or unwanted.19 Additionally, teenagers reporting first sex as unwanted are more likely than others to report that their first partner had been three or more years their senior.20

Youth who become involved with a boyfriend or girlfriend may have more risky attitudes and behaviors than those who do not, even before starting a relationship. Some youth may put themselves in situations or associate with friends who encourage early dating or early sex. Others may actively seek out a boyfriend or girlfriend. For example, females who mature physically may be more interested in or more attractive to males than their peers; they may also be more interested in sex. Teenage women who report some of the common causes of adolescent problem behavior (including delinquency, difficulty in school and substance use) also report entering into relationships with older boyfriends.21

Some evidence suggests that Hispanic culture condones relationships between younger women and older men.22 In Hispanic culture, men are expected to have a dominating role in sexual situations, and this may be easier when the age difference between the man and the woman is large.

Using a longitudinal sample of urban, ethnically diverse middle school students, the analyses below attempted to answer five questions: What is the association between seventh-grade relationship status (i.e., having had an older, same-age or no boyfriend or girlfriend) and engaging in sexual behavior in the ninth grade? Do sixth-grade characteristics explain seventh-grade relationship status? Does relationship status in seventh grade explain eighth-grade psychosocial characteristics? Does the association between seventh-grade relationship status and ninth-grade sexual activity remain after sixth-grade characteristics are controlled for? Do eighth-grade psychosocial characteristics mediate the association between seventh-grade relationship status and ninth-grade sexual activity?

METHODS

Study Design

The data for this study come from a randomized controlled trial testing the effects of a middle school intervention to reduce sexual risk behaviors. The initial study involved 19 predominantly Hispanic middle schools (10 treatment and nine control) and 2,829 students (1,412 treatment and 1,417 control) in an urban area of Northern California. Students represented a mix of racial and ethnic backgrounds: Fifty-nine percent were Hispanic, 17% white, 16% Asian, 5% black and 3% other races or ethnicities. Paper-and-pencil surveys were administered in school each spring from 1997 to 2000, beginning when participants were in sixth grade. Students no longer attending a study school during follow-up were surveyed by mail. Ninety-one percent of the baseline sample were followed up in grade seven, 88% in grade eight and 64% in grade nine. (Follow-up of ninth graders was difficult because students were no longer in the same schools.) More information on the full sample, response rates and methods has been published elsewhere.23

To examine the impact of having an early romantic relationship on subsequent sexual behavior, we restricted our sample to the 2,269 students (1,070 males and 1,199 females) who indicated in seventh grade that they had never had sex. (In all, 2,573 students were surveyed in seventh grade. Of these, 13% of males and 8% of females reported sexual experience; 3% of males and 1% of females failed to respond to the question about sexual behavior.) The racial background of our sample paralleled that of the main study.

Instrument

The survey instrument was based on a theoretical model developed by the authors to guide the design of the prevention curriculum. It contained 100 questions tapping such areas as demographic characteristics, self-efficacy to resist sexual advances, peer norms regarding sexual behavior, reasons for having or not having sex, knowledge about HIV and condoms, and coercive and noncoercive sexual behaviors. The instrument was developed in English and translated into Spanish; a back-translation and decentering procedure was used to assure that the English and Spanish versions were equivalent.24

•Seventh-grade relationship status.Students were asked "Have you ever had a serious boyfriend or girlfriend?" and "How old was your oldest serious boyfriend or girlfriend?" Responses to the latter question were "I've never had a serious boyfriend or girlfriend," "10 years old or younger," "11 years old" and so forth to "18 years old or older." Responses were categorized as no relationship if the student reported never having had a serious boyfriend or girlfriend, as same-age boyfriend or girlfriend if the boyfriend or girlfriend was less than two years older, and as older boyfriend or girlfriend if the boyfriend or girlfriend was two or more years older than the respondent. Two or more years was chosen because in middle school, even this small difference has been found to be important.25

In analyses where relationship status was an explanatory variable, we used adjacent backward differences coding (older vs. same-age and same-age vs. no relationship) rather than dummy variable coding with a single reference level. This was accomplished by using Stata command xi3 with the b option.26

•Ninth-grade sexual behavior. In ninth grade, students were asked "Have you had sex in the last 12 months?" The phrase "having sex" was defined as "man's penis in a woman's vagina." To avoid identifying illegal sexual behavior that would have had to be reported, we did not ask students the age of any of their sexual partners or whether they had had sex with a "serious boyfriend or girlfriend." We did not explicitly measure same-gender sexual behavior.

•Psychosocial variables. Using measures assessed in both sixth and eighth grades, we created four psychosocial scales: unwanted sexual advances, peer norms regarding sex, situations that could lead to sex and sexual limits.

For unwanted sexual advances, we averaged responses to survey items asking whether, in the last 12 months, someone had tried to "kiss" the respondent, "tongue or French kiss" the respondent or "touch [the respondent's] private parts below the waist [defined as the penis or the vagina]" when the respondent did not want to be kissed or touched (alpha=0.64 for both males and females in sixth grade; 0.73 for males and 0.79 for females in eighth grade). Possible responses were "no," "yes, someone tried, but it didn't happen" and "yes, someone tried and it happened"; a response of "no" was coded 0, and either of the others was coded 1.

We measured peer norms by averaging responses to six items that assessed perceptions of peers' sexual behavior and attitudes—e.g., "How many of your friends have had sex?" and "How many of your friends think that people your age should wait until they're older to have sex?" (alpha=0.66 for males and 0.68 for females in sixth grade; 0.80 and 0.79, respectively, in eighth grade). The five-point response scale (1–5) ranged from "none" to "all."

To measure situations that could lead to sex, we averaged responses to four items: how often in the past three months respondents had been alone with someone they were very attracted to, had kissed and touched someone they were very attracted to, had lain on a couch or bed alone with someone they really liked, and had been at a party with no adults in the house (alpha=0.69 for each gender in sixth grade; 0.82 for each in eighth grade). Possible responses (coded from 1 to 4) were "never," "1–2 times," "3 to 5 times" and "6 or more times."

The sexual limits measure combined responses to items asking if students would permit someone they liked very much to engage in four behaviors, ranging from "kiss you on the lips" to "have sex with you" (alpha=0.81 for males and 0.71 for females in sixth grade; 0.81 and 0.86, respectively, in eighth grade). Responses (coded from 1 to 4) ranged from "definitely yes" to "definitely not."

To address the skewed shape of and gaps in the distri-bution of these four scales, we used an ordinal version rather than the actual values. For three of the four, we used categorization at the five quintile points. The unwanted sexual advances scale did not vary enough, so it was categorized into four levels.

•Ethnicity. For their ethnicity, respondents could mark as many of the following categories as applied: Hispanic/ Latino/Mexican; black/African American; Asian/Pacific Islander; American Indian/Alaskan Native; white/Caucasian; and other. For these analyses, students who identified themselves as Hispanic in sixth or seventh grade were compared with all others.

•Menarche. Females in sixth grade were asked whether they had had their first period or menstruation; those answering "yes" (coded 1) were compared with those answering "don't know" or "no" (coded 0). Males were not asked a question about sexual maturation, because no clear demarcation is available.

Analytic Plan

We used a variety of analytic techniques to examine our five research questions. All analyses were performed separately for each gender.

•Association between seventh-grade relationship status and ninth-grade sexual behavior. Using logistic regression, we regressed sexual behavior in the ninth grade on seventh-grade relationship status.

•Association between sixth-grade characteristics and seventh-grade relationship status. Using multinomial logistic regression, we regressed seventh-grade relationship status individually on sixth-grade psychosocial variables, Hispanic ethnicity and sixth-grade menarche. The resulting relative risk ratios combine in a single model the odds ratios for having had a same-age boyfriend or girlfriend versus no relationship, and the odds ratios for having had an older versus a same-age boyfriend or girlfriend. Instead of two binary logistic regression models on parts of the data, all of the data are used for the estimation, resulting in better statistical efficiency.27

•Association between seventh-grade relationship status and eighth-grade characteristics. Using linear regression, we regressed eighth-grade psychosocial variables individually on seventh-grade relationship status, controlling for the corresponding sixth-grade psychosocial variables.

•Effects of controlling for sixth-grade characteristics. Using logistic regression, we regressed ninth-grade sexual be- havior on seventh-grade relationship status, controlling for sixth-grade psychosocial variables, Hispanic ethnicity and menarche.

•Mediating effects of eighth-grade characteristics. Using logistic regression, we regressed ninth-grade sexual behavior on seventh-grade relationship status, controlling for eighth-grade and sixth-grade psychosocial variables.

For all analyses, we used two-tailed tests and a 5% level of significance. All regression models included the variable "treatment versus control schools," which was not statistically significant in any of the analyses. These analyses were not meant to assess the impact of the intervention on initiation of sex; the results of an impact analysis are presented elsewhere.28

Because of attrition, and because some respondents did not answer all questions, substantial amounts of data would have been lost if we had used casewise deletion of observations with missing data. To the extent that the data were not missing completely at random, or at least approximately so, attrition could bias the results. To address this threat to validity, we used multiple imputation techniques for all analyses.29 Twenty data sets were imputed using SAS PROC MI on data from the full sample; because PROC MI assumes a multivariate normal distribution, imputed values for binary and ordinal variables were rounded to the nearest applicable integer.30

Imputed data sets were generated separately for males and for females, to accommodate the need for the female-specific variable (menarche). To create the 20 data sets used in the analyses, all outcomes and explanatory variables described in this article were included. Using the Stata command micombine,31 we fitted the same model to each data set; using special formulas, we combined parameter estimates from the fitted models into the overall parameter estimates and their standard errors. The significance tests were performed for the overall parameter estimates. The Ns in Table 1 are averaged over the 20 multiply imputed data sets.

RESULTS

In seventh grade, about half of both males and females in the sample reported never having had a serious boyfriend or girlfriend (Table 1). Six percent of males and 18% of females reported having had a boyfriend two or more years their senior.

•Association between seventh-grade relationship status and ninth-grade sexual behavior. One in 10 males and females who reported no relationship in seventh grade said that they had had sex in the 12 months before the ninth-grade interview, compared with at least one in five who reported in seventh grade having had a same-age or older girlfriend or boyfriend (Table 2). Results of the logistic regression analysis confirm that compared with students who initially reported no relationship, those who had had a relationship with someone their age had more than twice the odds of being sexually active in ninth grade (odds ratio, 2.1 for males and 2.9 for females). Females who had had an older boyfriend by seventh grade were more likely than those who had had a same-age boyfriend to report sexual activity in ninth grade (2.1).

•Association between sixth-grade characteristics and seventh-grade relationship status. Youth who reported unwanted sexual advances in sixth grade were more likely to have had a same-age boyfriend or girlfriend than to have had none by seventh grade (relative risk ratio, 1.8 for each gender—Table 3), and more likely to have had an older boyfriend or girlfriend than to have had one of their age (1.4 for each). Findings were similar for youth who reported having peers with more permissive sexual norms in the sixth grade and for those who reported more situations that could lead to sex. Youth with strong sexual limits in sixth grade were at reduced risk of having had a same-age boyfriend or girlfriend rather than none by seventh grade (0.6–0.7), and of having had an older boyfriend or girlfriend rather than one of their age (0.8 for each gender). In addition, females who had experienced menarche by the sixth grade and females who were Hispanic were more likely to have had older boyfriends by seventh grade than to have had same-age boyfriends. Compared with males of other ethnic backgrounds, Hispanic males were more likely to report a same-age girlfriend rather than none.

•Association between seventh-grade relationship status and eighth-grade characteristics. Large differences in mean scores on the eighth-grade psychosocial variables (0.6–1.5 points on four- and five-point scales) suggest that youth who had had no relationship by seventh grade differ substantially from those who had had an older boyfriend or girlfriend (Table 4). For both genders, having had a same-age boyfriend or girlfriend by seventh grade was associated with greater risk on all psychosocial outcomes in eighth grade than was having had no relationship. By contrast, having had an older, rather than a same-age, boyfriend or girlfriend by seventh grade was a much less consistent predictor of eighth-grade outcomes. It was significant for both males and females only with regard to riskier peer norms in eighth grade; for females, it also predicted unwanted sexual advances and involvement in situations that could lead to sex.

•Effects of controlling for sixth-grade characteristics. The first model shown in Table 5 repeats our findings on associations between seventh-grade relationship variables and ninth-grade sexual behavior. The second model shows that when sixth-grade variables were added, males who reported having had a same-age girlfriend in seventh grade, rather than none, no longer had significantly elevated odds of reporting sexual activity in ninth grade. Instead, sixth-grade peer norms and Hispanic ethnicity were significantly associated with reports of sexual activity in ninth grade. For females, when sixth-grade variables were added to the analysis, the effect of having had a same-age boyfriend, rather than none, was reduced but remained significant. Also, having had an older boyfriend rather than a same-age one was no longer significant for females. In this model, sixth-grade peer norms, sexual limits and menarche predicted ninth-grade reports of sexual activity.

•Mediating effects of eighth-grade characteristics. The final model in Table 5 shows that when eighth-grade variables were added to the previous model, seventh-grade relationship status did not predict sexual behavior in ninth grade for males or females. For males, sixth-grade peer norms and Hispanic ethnicity remained significant; eighth-grade situations that could lead to sex also predicted ninth-grade reports of sexual activity. For females, peer norms and situations that could lead to sex in eighth grade were associated with reported sexual behavior in ninth grade, and menarche in sixth grade remained significant.

DISCUSSION

Over the past decade, several studies have shown that adolescent and preadolescent females with an older boyfriend experience various types of sexual risk.32 Youth in middle school, particularly females who have had a same-age boyfriend and males, have not been studied as extensively. We have found that having had a boyfriend or girlfriend by seventh grade is both a predictor of having sex in the ninth grade and a marker of prior risks for sex. Patterns of results differed for males and females.

Males who had had a girlfriend by seventh grade were more likely than those who had not to be sexually active in ninth grade; however, the risk was the same for those who had had an older girlfriend and those whose girlfriend had been their age. Given that only 6% of males reported having had an older girlfriend by seventh grade, our analysis may have lacked sufficient power to assess the impact of older girlfriends. Furthermore, we had no information on the duration or seriousness of early relationships.

Males who reported a girlfriend in seventh grade were already on a trajectory leading to early sex. In sixth grade, they reported riskier peer norms, lower sexual limits, more situations that could lead to sex and more unwanted sexual advances than their peers who had not had a relationship. Without a biological indicator of puberty, it is impossible to assess the extent to which these differences might be due to early maturation.

Our study is one of the first to examine in depth the effects of having a girlfriend on males' later sexual behavior. Although only peer norms in sixth grade and situations that could lead to sex in eighth grade were significant in our multivariate analyses, they effectively controlled for and mediated the association between having a girlfriend in seventh grade and being sexually active in ninth grade. Males who reported being in more situations that could lead to sex in eighth grade may have had less parental monitoring or may have had a stronger interest in or more opportunities for sex because of physical maturity. More work is needed to determine if these associations are found in other populations.

It is striking that having peers who endorsed early sex in sixth grade would be predictive of having sex three years later. This may indicate the power of these early friendships or that such friendships are in part a marker for other family and community risk factors. If peer norms are directly linked to risk, parents, especially fathers, need to be vigilant about getting to know their sons' friends of both genders, as well as providing unambiguous messages about the inappropriateness of early sex.

Hispanic males in this sample were more likely than non-Hispanic males to have had a girlfriend by seventh grade and more likely to be sexually active in ninth grade. These findings are consistent with those of other studies. For example, in a study based on the Add Health data set, Hispanic males were more likely than white males and less likely than black males to report sex before age 15.33 Hispanic culture, including traditional gender roles, puts pressure on men to be sexually active,34 and this pressure is almost certainly transmitted to adolescent males.

By contrast, females who reported a same-age boyfriend in seventh grade were more likely than those reporting no boyfriend in seventh grade to say they had had sex in ninth grade, and those reporting older boyfriends in seventh grade were more likely than those reporting a same-age boyfriend to be sexually active in ninth grade. The differences are large (represented by odds ratios of 2.9 and 2.1, respectively) and important, and suggest that any "serious" relationship in seventh grade should be cause for concern. The increased risk associated with having a boyfriend was not limited to a small proportion of females: Half of females reported in seventh grade that they had had a serious boyfriend.

The one-fifth of females in the sample who had had an older boyfriend by seventh grade were at particularly high risk of being sexually active in ninth grade. Females might be attracted to older males for many reasons. For example, older men generally have more resources than younger men. They may also be more mature and have higher status in the eyes of younger females and their friends. Unfortunately, these same characteristics may create an imbalance of power and thereby increase the pressures for sex and unprotected sex.35 In addition, a younger teenager may be at a social and developmental disadvantage when dating someone several years older, making it even more difficult to refuse sexual advances. Finally, an older boyfriend is more likely than a younger one to be sexually experienced, and therefore is more likely to expect sex in a relationship. In fact, some older men may seek out younger partners precisely because they are more able to control younger partners and interactions with them.36

Parents should be warned of the risks of interactions between middle school females and older boyfriends, even when those boyfriends are only two years older. Programs and parents need to find creative ways to address the risks of having an older boyfriend without making such activities appear more attractive than they already are.

Females who have had boyfriends by the seventh grade are different from those who have not. Their riskier peer norms, lower sexual limits and greater likelihood of having experienced menarche in sixth grade partially explained the association between having had a boyfriend by seventh grade and ninth-grade sexual activity. As with males, having a romantic relationship in seventh grade may be a marker for a more general pattern of behavior and biology predisposing females to early sex. However, eighth-grade mediating factors of peer norms and situations that could lead to sex, along with early menarche, more fully explained the relationship between seventh-grade relationship status and ninth-grade sexual behavior.

Females who had physically matured early were at particular risk of having had an older boyfriend by seventh grade and of being sexually active in ninth grade, and this association was unchanged when the eighth-grade variables were included in the analysis. This suggests that early menarche is a unique risk factor, explaining variance unexplained by the eighth-grade psychosocial risk factors.

This finding is of concern because young females are experiencing menarche at increasingly early ages.37 Physical maturity does not equate to emotional, social and cognitive maturity, and young females who develop early may require special interventions or counseling to teach them how to handle both the sexual harassment common in grade schools38 and interactions with older males. During the testing of our intervention,39 many young people asked when they would be "ready for sex." It might be useful for interventions aimed at postponing sexual involvement to address the issue of physical maturity and "readiness" for sex, emphasizing that the two are not the same.

Having a serious boyfriend in seventh grade appears to make it more likely that females will put themselves in situations that may lead to sex in eighth grade. There are a number of ways that being in these situations might lead to sex. For one, kissing and touching are enjoyable and likely to increase curiosity and sexual desire. Also, once females are seen as having been romantically involved, they may get a reputation that encourages others to approach them, again placing them in situations that could lead to sex. To some extent, this reasoning also applies to males.

Youth who are poorly supervised and live in a permissive environment may be more likely than others to be in situations that could lead to sex. Less supervision in high school has been associated with sexual initiation, more sexual partners and (among males) more STDs.40 Discouraging dating and other social activities in middle school that make having a boyfriend or girlfriend look attractive and normative, and encouraging greater parental supervision, could be part of a comprehensive school-based sex education program to better address these issues.41

Although this study has a number of strengths, including its longitudinal design, some of its data should be viewed with caution. For example, the question about having an older or any boyfriend or girlfriend in seventh grade was quite general, and the analyses cannot determine if youth had had sex with that boyfriend or girlfriend or a different one. However, the strong association between that measure and sexual activity in the ninth grade suggests it is important. Another data limitation is the lack of any measure of early maturation for the males in this study. Only 75 males both had had an older girlfriend and were sexually inexperienced by seventh grade. Thus, conclusions about the effects of males' having an older girlfriend should be considered tentative. The relatively low response rate in ninth grade and lack of information about same-gender behavior are also limitations. On the positive side, the total sample was quite large, included both genders and represented an ethnically diverse population.

Given the complex biological, normative and behavioral issues, addressing the risk factors for early sexual involvement will be challenging. Helping girls to handle the social changes related to early pubertal development, deemphasizing social activities that may pave the way for risky behavior and encouraging parental supervision may help reduce early involvement with a boyfriend or girlfriend.

References

1. Manlove J et al., Sex between young teens and older individuals: a demographic portrait, Child Trends Research Brief, Washington, DC: Child Trends, 2005.

2. Landry D and Forrest J, How old are U.S. fathers? Family Planning Perspectives, 1995, 27(4):159–161 & 165; Males M and Chew K, The age of fathers in California adolescent births, 1993, American Journal of Public Health, 1996, 86(4):565–568; and Lindberg LD et al., Age differences between minors who give birth and their adult partners, Family Planning Perspectives, 1997, 29(2):61–66.

3. Lindberg LD et al., 1997, op. cit. (see reference 2).

4. Boyer CB et al., Sexually transmitted diseases in a health maintenance organization teen clinic: associations of race, partner's age and marijuana use, Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 1999, 153(8):838–844.

5. Begley E et al., Older partners and STD prevalence among pregnant African American teens, Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 2003, 30(3):211–213.

6. DiClemente RJ et al., Sexual risk behaviors associated with having older sex partners: a study of black adolescent females, Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 2002, 29(1):20–24; Miller KS, Clark LF and Moore JS, Sexual initiation with older male partners and subsequent HIV risk behavior among female adolescents, Family Planning Perspectives, 1997, 29(5):212–214; and Manlove J et al., 2005, op. cit. (see reference 1).

7. Manlove J et al., 2005, op. cit. (see reference 1).

8. Kaestle CE, Morisky DE and Wiley DJ, Sexual intercourse and the age difference between adolescent females and their romantic partners, Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 2002, 34(6):304–309.

9. Abma JC et al., Teenagers in the United States: sexual activity, contraceptive use and childbearing, 2002, Vital and Health Statistics, Series 23, No. 24, 2004.

10. Leitenberg H and Saltzman H, A statewide survey of age at first intercourse for adolescent females and the age of their male partners: relation to other risk behaviors and statutory rape implications, Archives of Sexual Behavior, 2000, 29(3):203–215.

11. Blum RW, Beuhring T and Rinehart PM, Protecting Teens: Beyond Race, Income and Family Structure, Minneapolis: Center for Adolescent Health, University of Minnesota, 2000.

12. Halpern CT et al., Smart teens don't have sex (or kiss much either), Journal of Adolescent Health, 2000, 26(3):213–225.

13. Little CB and Rankin A, Why do they start? explaining reported early-teen sexual activity, Sociological Forum, 2001, 16(4):703–729.

14. Marín BV et al., Older boyfriends and girlfriends increase risk of sexual initiation in young adolescents, Journal of Adolescent Health, 2000, 27(6):409–418.

15. Blum RW, Beuhring T and Rinehart PM, 2000, op. cit. (see reference 11); and Halpern CT et al., 2000, op. cit. (see reference 12).

16. Young AM and D'Arcy H, Older boyfriends of adolescent females: the cause or a sign of the problem? Journal of Adolescent Health, 2005, 36(5):410–419.

17. Marín BV et al., 2000, op. cit. (see reference 14); Carvajal SC et al., Psychosocial predictors of delay of first sexual intercourse by adolescents, Health Psychology, 1999, 18(5):443–452; and Resnick MD et al., Protecting adolescents from harm: findings from the National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health, Journal of the American Medical Association, 1997, 278(10):823–832.

18. Raymond Associates, Teen partners study: final report, Rochester, NY: Raymond Associates, 1996; and Elstein SG and Davis N, Sexual relationships between adult males and young teen females: exploring the legal and social responses, Chicago: American Bar Association, Center on Children and the Law, 1997.

19. Moore KA, Nord CW and Peterson JL, Nonvoluntary sexual activity among adolescents, Family Planning Perspectives, 1989, 21(3): 110–114; Moore KA, Driscoll AK and Lindberg LD, A statistical portrait of adolescent sex, contraception, and childbearing, Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy, 1998; and Abma JC et al., 2004, op. cit. (see reference 9).

20. Abma JC et al., 2004, op. cit. (see reference 9).

21. Young AM and D'Arcy H, 2005, op. cit. (see reference 16).

22. de Anda D, Becerra RM and Fielder E, In their own words: the life experiences of Mexican-American and white pregnant adolescents and adolescent mothers, Child and Adolescent Social Work, 1990, 7(4):301–318; and Reynoso T, Felice M and Shragg P, Does American acculturation affect outcome of Mexican-American teenage pregnancy? Journal of Adolescent Health, 1993, 14(4):257–261.

23. Coyle K et al., Draw the Line/Respect the Line: a randomized trial of a middle school intervention to reduce sexual risk behaviors, American Journal of Public Health, 2004, 94(5):843–851.

24. Marín G and Marín B, Research with Hispanic Populations, Newbury Park, CA: Sage Press, 1991.

25. Marín BV et al., 2000, op. cit. (see reference 14).

26. UCLA Academic Technology Service, Stata FAQ: how do I use xi3? , accessed Mar. 7, 2006.

27. Begg CB and Gray R, Calculation of polychotomous logistic regression parameters using individualized regressions, Biometrika, 1984, 71(1):11–18; and StataCorp, Stata Statistical Software: Release 8, College Station, TX: StataCorp, 2003.

28. Coyle K et al., 2004, op. cit. (see reference 23).

29. Rubin D, Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys, New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1987; Schafer J, Analysis of Incomplete Multivariate Data, London: Chapman & Hall, 1997; Royston P, Multiple imputation of missing values, Stata Journal, 2004, 4(3):227–241; van Buuren S, Boshuizen HC and Knook DL, Multiple imputation of missing blood pressure covariates in survival analysis, Statistics in Medicine, 1999, 18(6):681–694; van Buuren S, Multiple Imputation Online, , accessed Mar. 7, 2006; and Li K, Raghunathan T and Rubin D, Large sample significance levels from multiply-imputed data using moment-based statistics and an F reference distribution, Journal of the American Statistical Association, 1991, 86(416):1065–1073.

30. SAS Institute, SAS OnlineDoc, Version 9, Cary, NC: SAS Institute, 2004.

31. Royston P, 2004, op. cit. (see reference 29).

32. Manlove J et al., 2005, op. cit. (see reference 1); Landry D and Forrest J, 1995, op. cit. (see reference 2); Males M and Chew K, 1996, op. cit. (see reference 2), Lindberg LD et al., 1997, op. cit. (see reference 2); Boyer CB et al., 1999, op. cit. (see reference 4); Begley E et al., 2003, op. cit. (see reference 5); DiClemente RJ et al., 2002, op. cit. (see reference 6), Miller KS, Clark LF and Moore JS, 1997, op. cit. (see reference 6); and Kaestle CE, Morisky DE and Wiley DJ, 2002, op. cit. (see reference 8).

33. Bruckner H and Bearman P, Dating behavior and sexual activity of young adolescents: analyses of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, in: Albert B, Brown S and Flanigan CM, eds., 14 and Younger: The Sexual Behavior of Young Adolescents, Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy, 2003.

34. Marín BV and Gómez CA, Latinos and HIV: cultural issues in AIDS prevention, in: Cohen PT, Sande MA and Volberding PA, eds., The AIDS Knowledge Base, third ed., Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 1999, pp. 917–923; and Marín BV et al., Condom use in unmarried Latino men: a test of cultural constructs, Health Psychology, 1997, 6(5):458–467.

35. Montfort S and Brick P, Unequal partners: teaching about power and consent in adult-teen relationships, Morristown, NJ: Planned Parenthood of Greater Northern New Jersey, 2000.

36. Ibid.; and Raymond Associates, 1996, op. cit. (see reference 18).

37. Wattigney W et al., Secular trend of earlier onset of menarche with increasing obesity in black and white females: the Bogalusa Heart Study, Ethnicity and Disease, 1999, 9(2):181–189.

38. Kopels S and Dupper D, School-based peer sexual harassment, Child Welfare, 1999, 78(4):435–460; and Fineran S and Bennett L, Teenage peer sexual harassment: implications for social work practice in education, Social Work, 1998, 43(1):55–64.

39. Coyle K et al., 2004, op. cit. (see reference 23).

40. Cohen D et al., When and where do youths have sex? the potential role of adult supervision, Pediatrics, 2002, 110(6):e66, , accessed Mar. 7, 2006.

41. Wilder EI and Watt TT, Risky parental behavior and adolescent sexual activity at first coitus, Milbank Quarterly, 2002, 80(3):481–524.

Author's Affiliations

Barbara VanOss Marín is a consultant, Division of Violence Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta. Douglas B. Kirby is senior research scientist, and Karin K. Coyle is director of research, ETR Associates, Scotts Valley, CA. Esther S. Hudes is specialist statistician, Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of California, San Francisco (UCSF). Cynthia A. Gómez is associate professor and codirector, UCSF Center for AIDS Prevention Studies.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by grant MH 51515 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The authors gratefully acknowledge the contributions of Romy Bernard, Nancy Calvin, Tiffany Chinn and Deborah Ivie to this research project. The project could not have been a success without the support of the participating district representatives, principals, teachers, school staff and students.

Disclaimer

The views expressed in this publication do not necessarily reflect those of the Guttmacher Institute.